The Psychology of Anchoring and Adjustment

The psychology of anchoring and adjustment can undermine our spending decisions, negotiations and relationships. But it’s also an underestimated opportunity.

Memories are tricky little things. They have a downright unfair relationship with time.

When our days pass without distinguishing characteristics from one another, time gets turbo charged. That forgettable office routine blurs into a speedy haze of months and years without anything to connect the long-term memory dots.

Joshua Foer eloquently explains this phenomenon in Moonwalking with Einstein, a book about the science of memory and how someone ordinary (the author in this case) can develop extraordinary capacities to remember.

The trick, Foer suggests, is that we need “associational hooks” for memories to stick to. Novelty makes these hooks all the more effective.

When you think about how you might remember in the future, this is why particular months of 2020 may serve a universal recall role for billions. As we try to recall life events that took place, their parallel position with the unusual events in the external world will make them easier to remember.

Like checkpoints on a long road, such anchors are dotted throughout the timelines of our lives. They serve as one of the most useful internal devices we have for keeping the past alive in our minds.

But anchors also have a near-term role, serving as an initial reference point for estimates and choices. And as psychologists have shown for decades, some of these anchors can be a force for irrationality.

Anchoring, Adjustment and Arbitrary Coherence

In 2003, Dan Ariely, George Loewenstein and Drazen Prelec published an intriguing study in The Quarterly Journal of Economics. In a series of six experiments, they wanted to explore how arbitrary anchors could influence unrelated decisions.

Imagine the scene. A professor takes out a bottle of wine and places it on a table. Students are then asked to write down the last two digits of their social security number and decide if they would spend that amount on the wine. Then, in a subsequent auction, students are asked to bid on the wine. The same process is followed for five other products.

Here’s where the results get worrying. Students with higher social security digits bid nearly twice as much as students with lower social security digits. An arbitrary anchor seemed to be influencing bidding on completely unrelated objects – a phenomenon psychologists call arbitrary coherence.

So what was going on?

The students were exhibiting a psychological heuristic known as anchoring and adjustment. According to this heuristic, we start with a reference point (or anchor) and then make adjustments to that reference point based on additional information in order to reach our estimate or choice. As the students demonstrated, these adjustments are often insufficient and leave an estimation that gives the initial anchor undue influence.

The effect has been demonstrated across a multitude of studies. One of the seminal studies by Amos Tversky and Daniel Kahneman fixed a roulette wheel to only land on 10 or 65 and then asked participants to estimate the percentage of the United Nations that were African nations. Again, a completely unconnected anchor influenced decisions. Those who landed on 10 guessed on average 25% and those who landed on 65 guessed on average 45%.

Real-World Examples of Anchoring

In the real world, of course, we’re unlikely to play on roulette wheels and then take geopolitical trivia questions. And even if we did, why does this really matter?

The short answer is that cases of arbitrary coherence matter because they demonstrate that our choices are more malleable than we think. If we’re aware of this bias, we’re far less likely to fall into these traps, deliberately set or not.

But when they are not arbitrary, anchors can be equally – if not more – potent.

A few examples to illustrate this point:

#1: List prices: The recommended retail price (RRP) or list price of a product is an immediate anchor. Numerous studies have shown that when given different list prices, average estimates of the real value of items can be skewed. Anchoring and adjustment can therefore leave us overpaying or stretching our profits, depending on which side we fall on.

#2: Salary negotiations: The starting offer in salary negotiations serves as an anchor for subsequent offers. Studies suggest that there is stronger probability of a higher salary offer when starting with a more extreme higher anchor. (A practical word of caution here: absurd offers will be laughed out of town.)

#3: Investing: Investors may hold investments that have lost value because the original purchase price serves as an anchor. While additional information may factor in some adjustments, the original anchor can still exert significant influence over valuations.

#4: Medical diagnoses: Doctors are not immune to cognitive biases either. In a systematic review of the literature, one study concludes that first impressions can create diagnosis anchoring points in medicine, making it more difficult to diagnose.

The Value of First Information

Once we know about anchoring, we can better self-regulate against it. But it also offers some opportunities.

Think about that product you’re selling: is your list price (the anchor) high enough to make that discount appear more enticing? Or that salary negotiation: is your initial offer (the anchor) high enough to drive the most value from negotiations?

This extends to the simple question of first impressions. Individuals’ first impressions of us (the anchor) can take a long time to shift and adjust. It’s why many criminal defence lawyers ask their clients to wear glasses. And it’s why we wear a suit and tie for that job interview, which might otherwise only be dragged out of the wardrobe for an occasional wedding.

Bottom line: The first bit of information we receive leaves a mark, whether we like it or not. By understanding anchoring, we can either get on the right side of that bias or wilfully incur its costs.

The next time you are about to encounter a spending decision, a negotiation, or a first meeting, give that some thought. That first piece of information you receive and transmit may have longer lasting and more irrational effects than you originally thought.

Get More Insights Straight to Your Inbox

Sign up to the weekly newsletter and never miss a post again.

Sponsored links below (if any). Clicking is one way of saying thanks for the article!

Related Reading